‘Reducing risk behaviours after stroke’ You can view Patricia Hall’s latest publication here;
Reducing risk behaviours after stroke: An overview of reviews interrogating primary study data using the Theoretical Domains Framework
Patricia Hall, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, corresponding author 1 , 2 ,* Maggie Lawrence, Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing, 3 Thilo Kroll, Supervision, Writing – review & editing, 4 Catherine Blake, Writing – review & editing, 1 James Matthews, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing, 1 and Olive Lennon, Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing 1
Tinashe Mudzviti, Editor
Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer
Associated Data
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Go to:
Abstract
Background
Lifestyle changes, in addition to preventive medications, optimise stroke secondary prevention. Evidence from systematic reviews support behaviour-change interventions post-stroke to address lifestyle-related risk. However, understanding of the theory-driven mediators that affect behaviour-change post-stroke is lacking.
Methods
Electronic databases MEDLINE, Embase, Epistemonikos and Cochrane Library of Systematic Reviews were searched to March 2023 for systematic reviews addressing behaviour-change after stroke. Primary studies from identified systematic reviews were interrogated for evidence supporting theoretically-grounded interventions. Data were synthesized in new meta-analyses examining behaviour-change domains of the Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) and secondary prevention outcomes.
Results
From 71 identified SRs, 246 primary studies were screened. Only 19 trials (N = 2530 participants) were identified that employed theoretically-grounded interventions and measured associated mediators for behaviour-change. Identified mediators mapped to 5 of 14 possible TDF domains. Trial follow-up ranged between 1–12 months and no studies addressed primary outcomes of recurrent stroke or cardiovascular mortality and/or morbidity. Lifestyle interventions targeting mediators mapped to the TDF Knowledge domain may improve the likelihood of medication adherence (OR 6.08 [2.79, 13.26], I2 = 0%); physical activity participation (OR 2.97 [1.73, 5.12], I2 = 0%) and smoking cessation (OR 10.37 [3.22, 33.39], I2 = 20%) post-stroke, supported by low certainty evidence; Lifestyle interventions targeting mediators mapping to both TDF domains of Knowledge and Beliefs about Consequences may improve medication adherence post-stroke (SMD 0.36 [0.07, 0.64], I2 = 13%, very low certainty evidence); Lifestyle interventions targeting mediators mapped to Beliefs about Capabilities and Emotions domains may modulate low mood post-stroke (SMD -0.70 [-1.28, -0.12], I2 = 81%, low certainty evidence).
Conclusion
Limited theory-based research and use of behaviour-change mediators exists within stroke secondary prevention trials. Knowledge, Beliefs about Consequences, and Emotions are the domains which positively influence risk-reducing behaviours post-stroke. Behaviour-change interventions should include these evidence-based constructs known to be effective. Future trials should address cardiovascular outcomes and ensure adequate follow-up time.
Go to:
Introduction
Stroke, the leading cause of adult acquired disability, has a 20% risk of recurrence within five years [1]. A quarter of these recurrent events occur in the first year post-stroke, with an average annual risk thereafter of 4% [2]. Poorer prognosis and greater disability are associated with recurrent events, highlighting the need for effective risk reduction measures [1, 3]. Population attributable, modifiable risk factors account for up to 90% of stroke risk [4]. These include: hypertension, physical activity, dyslipidaemia, diet, central adiposity, psychosocial factors (home and/or work stress, life events and depression), current smoking, cardiac causes, high/heavy episodic alcohol consumption and diabetes mellitus. Modifiable risk factors for (recurrent)stroke are amenable to lifestyle change [5]. A modelling study suggests that lifestyle changes in diet and physical activity levels alongside optimized pharmacotherapy, could reduce five-year recurrent event rates after stroke by 80% [6]. Initiating and sustaining lifestyle changes to mitigate the risk of recurrent events constitute complex behaviours entailing multiple interacting components at individual, social and environmental levels [3, 7, 8]. Aspects such as knowledge, intentions, self-efficacy, motivation, outcome expectancies, perceived susceptibility/severity and social influences can act as barriers or facilitators to effective change [9]. These are classified as determinants of behaviour and can act as mediators for change, defined as ‘the intermediary variables in the causal process between an intervention and the behaviour change effect’ [10].
Whilst published SRs broadly address the efficacy of lifestyle-based interventions post-stroke [11–15], uncertainty remains about what works and why. Inclusion of studies lacking theory and understanding of determinants of health-related behaviours in stroke, may have influenced their findings. Failing to critically examine the role of these determinants as the mediators to affect behaviour, limits our understanding of effective interventions in stroke secondary prevention. An aligned overview of reviews [16], providing a best-evidence synthesis of behaviour-change interventions after stroke, highlighted the further need to delineate theory-based interventions and provide greater insight into why some interventions identified were effective or ineffective. This requires a better understanding of the role of theory and associated mediators in affecting behaviour-change [17, 18]. International secondary prevention guidelines recommend using theory-based lifestyle interventions [19], mirrored in the Medical Research Council (MRC) guidance on complex interventions where program theory is deemed essential to maximize the efficiency, use and impact of behaviour-change research [20].
Explicit use of theory in the design and evaluation of interventions in stroke secondary prevention presents this opportunity to understand why interventions work, for whom, and in what context [18]. Selecting one or more theories as the basis for intervention development can prove challenging, partly due to often overlapping theoretical constructs [21]. The Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) is a comprehensive theory-informed approach to understanding the determinants of behaviour change and the factors influencing intervention development. The TDF was developed to allow theories and their constructs to be synthesised into groupings to make behaviour-change theories more accessible in intervention design and analysis [21, 22]. This is important as it provides a systematic and rigorous framework for understanding behaviour change in multiple populations and settings. Comprising 87 component parts across fourteen overarching domains of Knowledge, Skills, Social/Professional Role and Identity, Beliefs about Capabilities, Optimism, Beliefs about Consequences, Reinforcement, Intentions, Goals, Memory, Attention and Decision Processes, Environmental Context and Resources, Social Influences, Emotions, and Behavioural Regulation [22], the TDF provides comprehensive coverage of the possible mediators influencing behaviour-change. Tabular representation of the TDF describes these domains as they pertain to stroke secondary prevention in the current study (S1 Table).
This overview of reviews aims to unpack complex interventions identified in primary studies across published SRs addressing behaviour-change post-stroke, to enable a better understanding of the underlying factors necessary to achieve the desired outcomes. Included primary studies are interrogated to identify the role of theory and proposed mediators for change, as mapped to the TDF domains. New meta-analyses synthesize primary studies by behaviour-change theoretical domains and secondary prevention outcomes, allowing the certainty of existing evidence supporting the working components of interventions to be examined. To our knowledge, the TDF has not previously been used as the basis from which to understand the behaviour-change constructs of effective stroke secondary prevention interventions.
Specific objectives
Provide a compendium of theoretically-grounded primary studies identified across published SRs addressing secondary stroke prevention, using behavioural/self-management interventions.
Extract the active components of reported interventions under the Template for Intervention description and Replication (TIDieR) [23] checklist, notably the theoretical perspectives described and the mediator/s for behaviour-change identified and measured.
Synthesize the results from the identified primary studies by behaviour-change theoretical domains and secondary prevention outcomes.
Determine the quality of evidence supporting the behaviour-change interventions and their working components by assigning a Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) [24] of evidence for meta-analyses conducted.
Go to:
Methods
This overview of reviews adheres to the preferred reporting guideline for overviews of reviews of healthcare interventions (PRIOR) statement and checklist (S2 Table) [25]. It was preceded by an a priori published protocol [26] which detailed a two-phased approach. Phase one, published elsewhere [16], identifies, synthesises and provides GRADE [24] of certainty for published meta-analytic evidence. Phase 2, reported here, first identifies primary study level data from all published SRs employing theoretically-grounded behaviour-change and/or self-management interventions and then synthesises these data in new meta-analyses to examine evidence supporting the mediators for behaviour-change in affecting positive changes in secondary prevention outcomes. A theoretically-grounded study was considered, for the purposes of this review, to be one that identified a theoretical perspective and identified and measured mediator/s for the targeted behaviour-change [27] that mapped to the TDF domains.
Inclusion/Exclusion criteria
SRs of randomized control trials (RCTs) or cluster RCTs (CRCT) testing interventions for behaviour-change and/or self-management of risk in stroke secondary prevention were first identified. Primary studies included in these reviews were then considered where the following were detailed:
Adult population comprising stroke/TIA
Intervention/s targeting stroke risk reduction at an individual or population level
Intervention/s identifying a theoretical perspective and measuring a stated mediator for behaviour-change that mapped to the TDF
Comparators of usual care, placebo, sham, or other intervention
Outcomes recorded that addressed mortality, recurrent stroke or other cardiovascular events, or secondary outcomes addressing any one or combination of the following health behaviours–secondary prevention medication adherence, healthy diet, physical activity participation, smoking cessation, safe alcohol consumption and emotional self-regulation.
Exclusion criteria applied:
Interventions designed to alter care process or health professionals’ education/practice.
Interventions not targeting behaviour-change in stroke secondary prevention.
Telehealth interventions
Interventions targeting family/partner dyads, unless behaviour-change in the person with stroke was specifically targeted and extractable.
Search strategy
Using a comprehensive search strategy compiled in conjunction with a liaison librarian, electronic databases MEDLINE, Embase, Epistemonikos and Cochrane Library of Systematic Reviews were systematically searched from inception to March 2023 with no limitations applied. For databases not specific to systematic reviews (Medline, Embase), a third methodological search string for systematic reviews was included. These two databases were chosen as they are two of the largest health focussed databases and we were confident, based on our experience and previous searches that they would contain the reviews we were looking for. In addition, reference lists of included SRs were checked. It is possible that more recent RCTs, not yet reviewed in SRs are not included. The full search strategy which targeted published systematic reviews is provided (S1 File).
Screening and selection
Identified SRs were screened at title and abstract stages by two independent reviewers. An inclusive approach was taken whereby if it was unclear whether the SR met the inclusion criteria, it progressed to the next stage. Full manuscripts were next reviewed independently for inclusion by two reviewers. SRs were included where they reported RCTs of behaviour-change/self-management interventions and reported secondary prevention outcomes of mortality, recurrent stroke, other cardiovascular events; health/lifestyle behaviours; emotional self-regulation; as summarized in the stroke secondary prevention model underpinning the overview (S1 Fig).
While phase 1 of this overview [16] excluded SRs that did not meet explicit criteria (i.e. no meta-analyses or where meta-analyses included mixed populations), phase 2 included any SR that contained primary RCTs of interest. All primary studies were subsequently screened for eligibility by two reviewers (PH, OL) to ensure they met the specific RCT study-level inclusion criteria listed above and that none of the following exclusion criteria applied:
Interventions designed to alter care process or health professionals’ education/practice.
Interventions not targeting behaviour-change in stroke secondary prevention.
Telehealth interventions
Interventions targeting family/partner dyads, unless behaviour-change in the person with stroke was specifically targeted and extractable.
Following screening of RCTs from identified SRs, each trial intervention, as described in the paper, was examined for inclusion against domains 1 and 2 of the TIDieR checklist [23]. These relate specifically to providing the rationale for the intervention and the theoretical perspective. Where theory and rationale were provided, the next level screening identified whether a mediator for behaviour-change, mapping to the TDF, was identified and measured. Studies that did not measure proposed mediator/s pre and post intervention were excluded. Any disagreements regarding study eligibility were resolved by discussion to reach consensus.
Data extraction
Data from included RCTs were independently extracted and cross-checked by two reviewers (PH, OL). These data included TIDieR checklist domains (1–10); mediators for behaviour-change; TDF domain of the mediator; participants’ characteristics (e.g. age, gender, time post-stroke); reported stroke secondary prevention outcomes.
Quality appraisal
The Cochrane Risk of Bias (ROB) [28] tool was used to assess the methodological quality of included RCTs. Where trials were appraised using this tool in the SR of origin, it was accepted and extracted by reviewers. Where more than one SR presented ROB for the same trial and a discrepancy between reviews was identified, the more conservative ROB was recorded. Where trials were appraised in the SR of origin using an alternative tool or where no appraisal was documented, two reviewers (PH, OL) independently appraised the trial using the Cochrane ROB tool to ensure conformity.
Data synthesis
Meta-analyses, conducted using Review Manager 5 (RevMan5) [29], grouped data by TDF domains and secondary prevention outcomes, where data presented permitted. For continuous data, where different scales assessed the same outcome, standardized mean differences (SMD) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated. SMD is used as a summary statistic to measure effect size that quantifies differences in standard deviations between two groups. The Hedges’ g version of SMD conducted here in RevMan5 is the preferred statistic when sample sizes are unequal and/or are small (< 20), as the case in the current study, as it takes each sample size into consideration when calculating the overall effect size. The inverse variance method was used as it is especially suitable when using SMD to minimise uncertainty of the overall effect size [30]. For dichotomous variables, odds ratios (OR) with 95% CIs were employed using the Mantel-Haenszel method. Random effects models were applied to provide a more conservative estimate of overall effect size as statistical heterogeneity was assumed [30]. The I2 statistic measured heterogeneity; >50% was considered substantial [30]. To over-come a unit-of-analysis error, where multiple comparisons from the same study were included more than once in a meta-analysis, the group was split into separate groups with smaller sample sizes to avoid overcounting [31]. Where included trials reported outcomes measured at more than one time point post-intervention, the last follow-up time was included in the meta-analysis. Where possible, sensitivity analysis was conducted, to examine pooled secondary prevention outcome data only from trials where the mediator for behaviour-change was observed to positively change. The certainty of the evidence for each effective intervention/outcome group identified was evaluated using the GRADE criteria [24] and agreed by consensus of two reviewers (PH and OL).
Go to:
Results
Study identification and selection
The PRISMA [32] flow diagram (Fig 1) details the overview flow process, including reasons for SR and subsequent primary RCT exclusion. As detailed, 71 reviews were included, yielding 246 unique RCTs. Of these, nineteen met the full criteria for inclusion [33–52]. Two papers which reported the same study at 3 and 12 months [49, 50] were considered as one trial. One study could not be retrieved, despite inter-library agency [53].
An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0302364.g001.jpg
Fig 1
PRISMA flow chart.
Description of included primary RCTs
Table 1 summarises the characteristics of all nineteen included RCTs. The trials were conducted across four geographical locations–Australia [33, 38, 40], North America [34, 36, 48], Asia [44, 45, 51, 52] and Europe [35, 37, 39, 41–43, 46, 47, 50]. When broken down by country and world bank classification all but two studies from an upper-middle-income economy (China) [51, 52] originated in high-income nations [54]. A total of 2530 participants with confirmed first or recurrent stroke/TIA were included across the trials. Seventeen trials included men and women, one included men only [41], and one did not report the gender of participants [35]. Usual care was the comparator in most trials; however, four trials compared the intervention to alternate/active controls (e.g. computerised cognitive training [41]; education program [47]; stress management program without mindfulness [51]; non-medication related conversation [43]. One trial was a three-arm trial [38] but only data from cognitive-behavioural therapy versus usual care is included here based on outcomes reported. Intervention follow-up ranged from immediate post-intervention to twelve months across the included RCTs.
Table 1
Study characteristics and findings.
Study Participants Intervention theoretical perspectives Brief description Mediator TDF Domain Intervention Time to follow-up Outcomes measured Key findings
Eames [33]
2013
Australia N = 77
I: n = 37, 55% female
Age: 55.2 (SD 16.7)
C: n = 40, 54% female
Age: 61.4 (SD 12.7) Health Belief Model Tailored stroke education and support package Stroke knowledge
No difference Knowledge Education and support package in addition to usual care;
Pre-discharge face-to-face session;
online information booklet;
verbal reinforcement 3 month follow-up Mood (HADS)
Self-efficacy Significantly better self-efficacy for accessing information
Evans-Hudnall [34]
2014
USA N = 52
I: n = 27, 42% female
Age: 56 (SD 9.9)
C: n = 25, 35% female
Age: 46.6 (SD 10.7) Cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) to facilitate behaviour change Tailored information and goal-setting self-care among underserved ethnic minority individuals Stroke knowledge
Positive change in intervention group Knowledge Secondary stroke prevention self-care education;
Pre-discharge face-to-face;
Post-discharge telephone follow-up 1 month follow-up Mortality
Medication adherence (no’s compliant)
Fruit & vegetable consumption
Exercise (minutes)
Tobacco & alcohol use Significantly reduced tobacco use and improved alcohol use
Gillham [35]
2010
UK N = 52
I: n = 26
Age: 67.7 (SD 12.0)
C: n = 26
Age: 68.9 (SD13.2) Transtheoretical Model Stages of Change with Motivational Interviewing techniques Enhanced secondary prevention education targeting readiness to change behaviour Readiness to Change
No difference Intentions Enhanced individual stroke risk factor education;
Initial post-stroke clinic interview using motivational interviewing techniques and telephone follow-up 3 month follow-up Mood (HADS)
Fruit & vegetable consumption
Exercise (frequency)
Alcohol use Significant change is self-reported diet and exercise behaviour
Green [36]
2007
Canada N = 52
I: n = 97, 42% female
Age: 66.3 (SD 12.4)
C: n = 100, 41% female
Age: 67.2 (SD 12.4) Transtheoretical Model Stages of Change with Motivational Interviewing techniques Educational counselling to increase stroke knowledge Readiness to Change
Shift from passive to active stage of change for both groups. Intentions One-to-one counselling on personal risk factors at initial post-stroke clinic;
Lifestyle class within 2 months 3 month follow-up Mortality
Stroke knowledge Significant improvement in stroke knowledge
Hjelle [37]
2019
Norway N = 322
I: n = 166, 40% female
Age: 66 (12.1)
C: n = 156, 42% female
Age: 65 (SD 13.3) Theory of Salutogenesis, Self-determination, Narrative Theories Dialogue-based intervention to enhance psychosocial well-being Sense of Coherence
No difference Emotions Dialogue-based individual sessions in participants’ home;
Commenced within 1 month of acute stroke;
Guided self-determination to empower decisions on psychosocial wellbeing 6 month follow-up Mortality
Normal Mood (GHQ)
Depression (Yale) Psychosocial wellbeing improved in both groups.
No significant benefit found
Hoffman(a) [38]
2015
Australia N = 33
I: n = 11, 36% female
Age: 63.6 (SD 13.0)
C: n = 10, 40% female
Age: 57 (SD 14.2) Self-efficacy and Motivational Interviewing Coping skills intervention to improve self-awareness and coping skills Self-efficacy
No difference Beliefs about Capabilities Cognitive behavioural coping skills approach;
face-to face sessions commenced pre-discharge and in participants’ home 3 month follow-up Mood (HADS)
Self-efficacy
Stroke knowledge No clear influence on anxiety and depression symptoms detected
Jones [39]
2016
UK N = 78
I: n = 40, 50% female
Age: 61.8 (SD 16.0)
C: n = 38, 34% female
Age: 68.8 (SD 10.3) Social cognition theory and self-efficacy principles Integrated stroke self-management programme Self-efficacy
No difference Beliefs about Capabilities Bridges Community Stroke Self-management;
Face-to-face home visits;
Workbook 3 month follow-up Mood (HADS)
Stroke self-efficacy No significant differences
Self-efficacy showed most sensitivity to change in intervention group
Kendall [40]
2007
Australia N = 100
I: n = 58, 29% female
Age: 66.4 (SD 10.9)
C: n = 42, 38% female
Age: 66.3 (SD 10.4) Stanford Model of Chronic Disease Self-management Psychosocial skill expansion using self-management education approach Self-efficacy
No difference Beliefs about Capabilities Community chronic disease self-management programme;
Additional stroke specific sessions;
Face-to-face community groups 3, 6, 6, 12 month follow-up Mood (stroke specific quality of life scale)
Self-efficacy No influence detected
Kootker [41]
2017 Netherlands N = 61
2 interventions (active control), all male, median age 61 years
I: n = 31
AC: n = 30 Cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) to facilitate change of irrational and negative thoughts Individually tailored cognitive behavioural therapy to reduce depressive symptoms Proactive coping competence
No between group difference.
Significant change in both groups Emotions CBT augmented with occupational /movement therapy versus
Computerised cognitive training 4, 8 month follow-up Mood (HADS)
Coping competence Significant and persistent improvement in anxiety & depression for both interventions.
CBT not superior
McKenna [42]
2015
NI, UK N = 25
I: n = 11, 36% female
Age: 62 (SD 13.5)
C: n = 13, 54% female
Age: 67.3 (SD 10.6) Self-efficacy principles Stroke self-management programme Self-efficacy
Positive change in intervention group Beliefs about Capabilities Bridges Community Stroke Self-management;
One-to-one sessions promoting specific behaviours;
Patient held workbook 3 month follow-up Mood (GHQ)
Self-efficacy
Stroke self-efficacy Less decline in mood in intervention group
O’Carroll [43]
2013
UK N = 58 with active control
I: n = 29, 31% female
Age: 68.4 (SD 11.3)
AC: n = 29, 41% female
Age: 70.7 (SD 10.5) Self-regulation theory; Implementations intention approach Brief intervention to increase medication adherence Beliefs about medications
Positive change in intervention group Beliefs about Consequences Cognitive/educational & behavioural brief intervention;
Face-to-face at home;
Control received visits and non-medication related conversations 3 month follow-up Medication adherence (MARS: MEMS)
Blood pressure
Beliefs about medications
Brief illness perception questionnaire Significantly higher adherence to medication, correct dose, taken on schedule
Sit [45]
2007
China N = 190
I: n = 107, 50% female
Age: 62.8 (SD 10.2)
C: n = 83, 37.5% female
Age: 64 (SD 12.0) Interactive learn-practise-feedback-learn approach Community-based interactive stroke prevention education programme Stroke knowledge
Positive change in intervention group Knowledge Community based group interactive education sessions to empower self-care 3 month follow-up Medication adherence
Salty food consumption
Exercise participation (type & frequency)
Stroke knowledge Significant positive changes in medication adherence, dietary habits, physical activity participation
Sit [44]
2016
China N = 210
I: n = 105, 47.6% female
Age: 67.8 (SD 14.2)
C: n = 105, 47.6% female
Age: 70.7 (SD 13.9) Theory of health empowerment; self-efficacy; self-management Stroke self-management programme to enhance patients knowledge and skills Self-efficacy
Positive change in intervention group Beliefs about Capabilities Health empowered stroke self-management programme in parallel with rehabilitation 6 month follow-up Mortality
Medication adherence
Self-BP monitoring
Self-efficacy No significant differences in medication adherence
Slark [46]
2013
UK N = 96
I: n = 47, 36% female
Age: 65 (SD 12.1)
C: n = 47, 47% female
Age: 66 (SD 12.7) MRC approach to complex interventions as a framework. Complex intervention designed to increase risk awareness and knowledge Enhanced individualised risk awareness intervention Stroke knowledge
Positive change in intervention group Knowledge Individual risk awareness session with tailored information;
Face-to-face as inpatient 3 month follow-up Medication adherence
Diet
Physical activity
Tobacco use
Alcohol use
Physiological factors
Stroke knowledge Significant increased risk awareness and self-reported lifestyle changes
Tielemans [47]
2015
Netherlands N = 113
2 interventions as active control
I: n = 58, 55% female
Age: 55.2 (SD 8.9)
AC: n = 55, 40% female
Age: 58.8 (SD 8.7) Self-management based on proactive coping and action planning versus
General Stroke Education programme Stroke specific self-management project teaching pro-active coping Proactive coping competence
No difference. Emotions Stroke specific self-management programme;
teaching proactive coping skills & action planning;
Face-to-face in small groups 9 month Mood (HADS)
Coping competence Did not favour self-management over education intervention
Towfighi [48]
2020
USA N = 100
I: n = 49, 41% female
Age: 60 (SD 7)
C: n = 51, 35% female
Age: 57 (SD 10) Healthy Eating and Lifestyle After Stroke (HEALS) conceptual model based on chronic disease self-management Enhanced self-management programme to improve diet and physical activity Readiness to Change
No difference Intentions Lifestyle management programme;
Health behaviours, risk factors, motivation addressed 6 month follow-up Fruit & vegetable consumption
Tobacco use
Physiological factors
Readiness to change No significant changes in outcomes, small effect sizes, a longer duration recommended
Wang [51]
2020
China N = 134, 54% female
Age: 59.9 (SD 10.6)
2 interventions as active control
I: n = 67
C: n = 67 Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy versus
Stress management/no mindfulness Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to improve quality of life and poststroke depression Trait mindfulness
Positive change in intervention group Emotions 2-hour group sessions of Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy over 8 consecutive weeks End of intervention Mood (CES-D)
Trait mindfulness (MAAS) Positive effects on patients’ depression, social well-being, and emotional well-being.
Watkins [49, 50]
2007, 2011
UK N = 411
I: n = 204, 42% female
Age: median 70 years
C: n = 207, 41% female
Age: median 70 years Motivational Interviewing (MI) Motivational interviewing to support and build patients’ motivation to adjust and adapt after stroke Beliefs and expectations of recovery
No difference Beliefs about Consequences early post-stroke MI sessions to improve mood;
Individual patient-centred counselling 3, 12 month follow-up Mortality
Mood (GHQ)
Beliefs and expectations Improved mood and reduced mortality
Zhang [52]
2015
China N = 89
I: n = 45, 35% female
Age: 58.7 (SD 9.2)
C: n = 44, 38% female
Age: 59.3 (SD 8.1) Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy Mindfulness-based behavioural training to improve poststroke depression Trait mindfulness
Positive change in intervention group Emotions Mindfulness-based cognitive behavioural training;
Intensive consolidation training End of intervention Mood (HAM-D)
Trait mindfulness (MAAS) Significantly decreased depression scores
Open in a separate window
I: Intervention; C: Control; TDF: Theoretical Domains Framework; BCTs: Behaviour change techniques; HADS: Hospital anxiety and depression scale; GHQ: General health questionnaire; CES-D: Centre for Epidemiological Studies-Depression; HAM-D: Hamilton depression rating scale; MARS: Medication adherence rating scale; MEMS: Medication event monitoring system; MAAS: Mindful attention awareness scale; MI: Motivational interviewing; MRC: Medical Research Council
The interventions, as described in each study, differed considerably in terms of their rationale, active components, delivery, and theoretical underpinning, as documented under all TIDieR domains (S3 Table). Secondary prevention outcomes that mapped to the prevention model (S1 Fig) [26] were extracted. Twelve stroke secondary prevention outcomes drawn from seven trials documented statistically significant effects. Significant heterogeneity of measures and scales employed across trials was evident, limiting meta-analyses that could be conducted.
Across the nineteen RCTs included, eight different mediators for behaviour-change were referenced and measured pre and post intervention. These mediators mapped to five of fourteen possible domains of the TDF, depicted in Fig 2. Eight trials [34, 42–46, 51, 52] demonstrated a positive change in the mediator for change between the intervention group and control, of which five reported statistically significant effects in outcomes related to stroke secondary prevention [34, 43, 45, 46, 52]. Others demonstrated improvement in both trial arms [36, 41].
An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0302364.g002.jpg
Fig 2
Theoretical Domains Framework, mediators, and included studies.
Quality appraisal
Risk of bias summary and graphical illustration of ROB assessments presented as percentages are included in supplemental material (S2 and S3 Figs). Principal sources of bias related to blinding of participants and personnel delivering interventions. As it is generally accepted that blinding of participants or interventionists would not be feasible in complex interventions, this criterion was not judged in the overall rating. Low ROB was judged where the criterion was met in remaining domains [46, 48]; an unclear or moderate risk was judged where there was low or unclear ROB for these domains [36, 37, 41, 43]; a high ROB was judged where there was high ROB in one or more domain [33–35, 38–40, 42, 44, 45, 47, 50–52].
Meta-analysis: Primary outcomes
Mortality, recurrent stroke, cardiovascular events Despite describing their interventions as designed to reduce risk, no included trial reported outcomes of cardiovascular death, recurrent stroke or cardiovascular events, and follow-up was short ranging between 1–12 months. Mortality data, available from five trials [34, 36, 37, 45, 50], was reported under adverse events. Pooled data on 1203 participants (S4 Fig), as grouped by TDF domain, demonstrated no significant difference in the odds of death as an adverse event in lifestyle-related behaviour-change interventions when compared to controls (OR 0.60, 95% CI 0.32–1.11, p = 0.62, I2 0%).
Meta-analysis: Secondary outcomes
Secondary outcomes addressed measures of lifestyle behaviour-change and emotional self-regulation. Seven trials [34, 35, 43–46, 48] reported outcomes of one or a combination of health behaviours addressing medication adherence, healthy eating, physical activity participation, tobacco and alcohol use. Twelve trials [23, 33, 35, 37, 39–42, 47, 50–52] reported emotional self-regulation outcomes utilizing measures of anxiety, depression or psychological/psychosocial distress. Table 2 provides the GRADE of evidence for all meta-analyses supporting stroke secondary prevention outcomes by theoretical domains.
Table 2
GRADE of evidence: Secondary prevention outcomes by theoretical domains.
Certainty assessment № of patients Effect Certainty
№ of studies Study design Risk of bias Inconsistency Indirectness Imprecision Other considerations behaviour change interventions usual care and/or active control Relative (95% CI) Absolute (95% CI)
Medication adherence/Knowledge domain—(frequency of adherence)
2 randomised trials seriousd not serious not serious seriousb none 58/74 (78.4%) 29/72 (40.3%) OR 6.08
(2.79 to 13.26) 401 more per 1,000
(from 250 more to 497 more) ⨁⨁◯◯
Low
Medication adherence/Knowledge and Beliefs about consequences domains (assessed with: MARS & MMAS)
2 randomised trials very seriousa not serious not serious seriousb none 136 112 – SMD 0.36 higher
(0.07 higher to 0.64 higher) ⨁◯◯◯
Very low
Physical activity participation/Knowledge domain—achieving targets
2 randomised trials seriousa not serious not serious seriousb none 98/154 (63.6%) 51/130 (39.2%) OR 2.97
(1.73 to 5.12) 26 more per 100
(from 14 more to 38 more) ⨁⨁◯◯
Low
Smoking cessation/Knowledge domain
2 randomised trials seriousc not serious not serious seriousb none 31/74 (41.9%) 7/72 (9.7%) OR 10.37
(3.22 to 33.39) 43 more per 100
(from 16 more to 69 more) ⨁⨁◯◯
Low
Depression/Beliefs about capabilities and Emotions domains(assessed with: HADS, HAMD, MAAS)
4 randomised trials very seriousa not serious not serious not serious none 159 151 – SMD 0.7 SD lower
(1.28 lower to 0.12 lower) ⨁⨁◯◯
Low
Open in a separate window
CI: confidence interval; OR: odds ratio; SMD: standardised mean difference
MMAS: Moriskey Medication Adherence Scale; MARS: Medication Adherence Rating Scale; HADS: Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; HAMD: Hamilton depression rating scale; MAAS: Mindful Attention Awareness Scale
Explanations:
a. high risk of bias in multiple domains assessed in Cochrane risk of bias tool
b. small number of included studies
c. studies were rated as unclear in multiple risk of bias domains and high risk of bias related to blinding
d. some concerns related to blinding
e. inconsistent effect direction across studies
Health behaviour outcomes
Medication adherence Five trials reported medication adherence as a risk reducing behavioural outcome [34, 43–46]. Data were pooled from two trials measuring frequency of self-reported adherence [34, 46]. Both trials used behaviour-change mediators mapped to the TDF Knowledge domain. Meta-analysis demonstrated a significant effect in favour of the intervention group (OR 6.08 [2.79, 13.26], p< 0.00001, I2 0%) (Fig 3a). GRADE criteria identified low certainty of evidence (Table 2). As both trials demonstrated a positive change in the measured mediator, no further sensitivity analysis was conducted.
An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0302364.g003.jpg
Fig 3
Forest plot: Lifestyle interventions versus usual care and/or active control for the outcome of medication adherence.
Three trials employed medication adherence scales: MMAS [44, 45]; MARS [43]. Data, as presented, permitted meta-analysis from two trials [43, 45] demonstrating a significant effect in favour of the intervention group (SMD 0.36 [0.07, 0.64], p = 0.01, I2 13%) and evidence of effect for the TDF domain of Beliefs about Consequences, drawn from one study [43] (Fig 3b). GRADE criteria identified very low certainty of evidence (Table 2). No sensitivity analysis was conducted as both included trials demonstrated a positive change in their identified mediator.
Diet Five trials reported healthy eating outcomes [34, 35, 45, 46, 48]. Data were pooled from three trials [34, 35, 48] with self-reported fruit and vegetable consumption outcomes and behaviour-change mediators mapped to TDF domains of Knowledge [34] and Intentions [35, 48]. Meta-analysis demonstrated no significant difference in fruit and vegetable consumption between groups (SMD -0.07 [-0.36, 0.21], p = 0.62, I2 0%) (S5 Fig). Sensitivity analysis, removing trials where no change in the mediator occurred [35, 48], left one remaining trial [34] that didn’t alter the findings (Z = 0.04, p = 0.97).
Two trials reported outcomes addressing salt or salty food consumption [45, 46] and used behaviour-change mediators mapped to the TDF Knowledge domain. The units or mode of measurement employed did not allow data to be pooled. One of these trials [45] demonstrated a significant reduction in salted food consumption in the intervention group (p = 0.004).
Physical activity participation Four trials reported physical activity participation outcomes [34, 35, 45, 46]. Data were pooled from two trials measuring self-reported physical activity levels [34, 35] and using behaviour-change mediators mapped to TDF domains of Knowledge [34] and Intentions [35]. Results demonstrated no significant effect in favour of the intervention group (SMD -0.07 [-0.46, 0.32], p = 0.72, I2 = 67%), however heterogeneity was substantial (S6 Fig). Sensitivity analysis, removing one trial where the measured mediator didn’t change [35], left one trial [34] that did not alter the findings (Z = 1.48, p = 0.14).
Data were pooled from two trials [45, 46] measuring self-reported achievement of physical activity targets and using behaviour-change mediators mapped to the TDF Knowledge domain. A significant effect in favour of the intervention was observed (OR 2.97 [1.73, 5.12], p< 0.0001, I2 0%)(Fig 4c). GRADE criteria identified Low certainty evidence (Table 2). No sensitivity analysis was conducted as both trials demonstrated positive changes in their measured mediator for behaviour-change.
An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0302364.g004.jpg
Fig 4
Forest Plot: Lifestyle interventions versus usual care for the outcome of achieving physical activity participation targets (panel c); Lifestyle interventions versus usual care and/or active control for the outcome of smoking cessation (panel d).
Smoking cessation Three trials addressed tobacco use post-stroke, employing behaviour-change mediators mapped to TDF domains of Knowledge [34, 46] and Intention [48]. Data were pooled for outcomes of smoking cessation rates post-intervention [34, 46]. Meta-analysis demonstrated a significantly higher likelihood of smoking cessation favouring the intervention group (OR 10.37 [3.22, 33.39], p < 0.0001, I2 20%) (Fig 4d). Low GRADE certainty evidence was identified (Table 2). Both trials demonstrated positive changes in the mediator measured, negating further sensitivity analysis.
Alcohol consumption Three trials reported safe alcohol consumption outcomes using behaviour-change mediators mapped to TDF domains of Knowledge [34, 46] and Intentions [35]. The units or mode of measurement employed in reported outcomes (alcohol abstinence, units consumed per week, percentage of self-reported alcohol reduction) did not allow data to be pooled. However, two trials demonstrating positive change in the mediator of Knowledge, reported significant alcohol abstinence rates (OR = 1.48, 1.36–1.53, p = 0.31) [34] and alcohol consumption reduction (OR = 4.48 [1.16, 17.29], p = 0.03) [46] in favour of the intervention group. The remaining trial [35], where the mediator mapped to the Intentions domain, demonstrated no effect.
Outcomes of emotional self-regulation
Anxiety Four trials reported outcomes addressing anxiety self-regulation as a risk reducing behaviour, using mediators mapped to the TDF domains of Knowledge [33], Beliefs about Capabilities [38, 39] and Emotions [41]. Data, as presented in these trials, permitted meta-analysis from 3 trials [33, 38, 39] using the HADS-Anxiety sub-scale with negative results (MD -0.68 [-1.63, 0.28], p = 0.26, I2 = 0%) (S7 Fig). No trial demonstrated a positive change in their identified mediator, negating further sensitivity analysis.
Depression Seven trials reported outcomes addressing self-regulation of low mood [33, 37–39, 41, 51, 52]. Data permitted meta-analysis from four trials using behaviour-change mediators mapped to TDF domains of Beliefs about Capabilities [38, 39] and Emotions [51, 52], demonstrating evidence of effect in favour of the intervention (SMD -0.70 [-1.28, -0.12], p = 0.02, I2 = 81%), with high heterogeneity (Fig 5e). Evidence of effect in the TDF Emotions domain, drawn from 2 trials (SMD -0.99 [-1.97, -0.00], p = 0.05, I2 = 91%) is also evident, however heterogeneity was considerable. Low GRADE certainty was applied to the main finding (Table 2). Sensitivity analysis, removing trials with no associated change in the mediator, left two remaining trials [51, 52] and did not alter the overall findings (Z = 1.96, p = 0.05, I2 = 91%).
An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0302364.g005.jpg
Fig 5
Forest plot: Lifestyle interventions versus usual care and/or active control for the outcome of depression self-regulation.
Psychological distress Seven trials reported outcomes addressing self-regulation of psychosocial distress, using behaviour-change mediators mapped to TDF domains of Beliefs about Capabilities [39, 40, 42], Beliefs about Consequences [50], Intentions [35] and Emotions [37, 47]. Data presented permitted meta-analysis from four trials with no evidence of significant effect evident (SMD -0.12 [-0.28, 0.05], p = 0.18, I2 = 0%) (S8 Fig). No trial demonstrated a positive change in the mediator measured, negating further sensitivity analysis.
Go to:
Discussion
Use of theory and theoretical constructs is recommended to enhance effectiveness of complex interventions [17, 55] and to allow greater understanding of behaviour-change processes [22]. This overview of reviews, providing new meta-analyses, summarises the quantity and quality of evidence supporting theoretically-grounded interventions for behavioural change and/or self-management of health behaviours in stroke secondary prevention. As multiple behaviour-change theories exist, the TDF proved a useful theoretical lens through which theory-associated mediators for behaviour-change post-stroke could be viewed. Formal mediation analysis (indirect and direct paths) was beyond the scope of this current work, rather the effect of proposed mediators associated with behaviour-change interventions was examined. In excluding RCTs unless a theoretical perspective and an identified and measured mediator for behaviour-change was provided, trials with inadequately described theoretical underpinnings were omitted. This addresses current uncertainty in published meta-analysis, where ineffectiveness or conflicting results may result from underuse of theory and/or a lack of understanding of determinants of health behaviours post-stroke.
Across the nineteen RCTs identified in this review, three TDF domains were associated with impactful health behaviour-change: Knowledge (increased stroke knowledge); Beliefs about Consequences (greater understanding of benefits/rationale for preventive actions); and Emotions (self-regulation of emotional responses). Indeterminate effects were found, with considerable heterogeneity, for two other TDF domains, of Beliefs about Capabilities (self-confidence, self-efficacy in ability to put knowledge to constructive use) and Intentions (conscious decision to perform a behaviour). The identification of only five TDF domains from a possible fourteen points to significant underutilisation and measurement of theory-associated mediators in current trials. As no trials addressing outcomes of cardiovascular morbidity, recurrent stroke, or other cardiovascular events in stroke secondary prevention met the inclusion criteria, evidence is still lacking for these primary outcomes. An important observation about the role of mediators in achieving health behaviour-changes post-stroke is evident across the meta-analyses reported. With the exception of the outcome of depression, all other secondary prevention outcomes demonstrating a positive effect (medication adherence, achievement of physical activity targets, smoking cessation), were drawn from trials where the mediator for change was positively influenced by the intervention. Similarly for negative meta-analyses related to fruit and vegetable consumption, anxiety and psychological distress outcomes, the proposed mediators for change did not improve in the included trials.
It is important to note commonalities and discrepancies that exist between the results of previously published SRs [11–15], a best evidence synthesis from these reviews [16] and the results presented here. High-level evidence of published meta-analyses addressing behavioural change and/or self-management interventions in stroke secondary prevention found only moderate overlap of primary studies between reviews, and identified the SRs as addressing different intervention types, broadly categorized as Multimodal; Behavioural change; Self-management and Psychological therapies [16]. This approach failed to delineate any theoretical overlap between the intervention types, or the specific mediators targeted in the interventions that were effective. The ability to replicate the intervention clinically and include the important theoretical domains by which to affect change remained challenging. This review now indicates that TDF domains of Knowledge, Beliefs about Consequences, and Emotions are important considerations for inclusion in secondary prevention interventions designed to achieve behaviour-change.
Current best-evidence identified in the TDF Knowledge domain is drawn from trials where mediators for behaviour-change included enhanced knowledge about the nature of stroke, risk factors, and consequences. Interventions provided interactive and tailored information on risk reducing behaviours; offered self-management information and support in goal setting to improve lifestyle habits; and provided information on self-monitoring in relation to maintaining behaviour-change. Four trials specified and measured stroke knowledge as the mediator for change associated with their intervention [33, 34, 45, 46]. Health behaviours that were successfully targeted through the mediator of stroke knowledge were medication adherence, physical activity participation, healthy eating, smoking and alcohol consumption. Published psychological determinants of medication adherence as a health behaviour, highlight the importance of knowledge for stroke survivors [56], noting greater knowledge is associated with better adherence to prescribed medication. Qualitative research further highlights that inadequate information about stroke and commonly prescribed preventive medications, pose significant patient-level barriers [57] and that passive written information provision was not a helpful means to improve adherence [58]. A recent scoping review identified a mismatch between guideline recommendations for behavioural counselling and that audited in clinical practice, where the latter largely comprised information provision only [59]. Making and sustaining behaviour-change is difficult to achieve. Information provision alone has been shown to be ineffective in affecting the mediator of knowledge and thus positive behaviour-change [60]. Active rather than passive information provision, mirrored in the interventions of the included studies in this review, has been found to be effective [61]. This current overview now provides definitive evidence to guide clinical practice in stroke secondary prevention in the use of active mechanisms of information provision, anchored in the Knowledge domain of the TDF.
Addressing individuals’ Beliefs about Consequences of stroke-related behaviours was identified in this overview as an important TDF domain that can influence adherence to preventive medications and target self-regulation of mood. This now constitutes an important domain to address in stroke secondary prevention interventions where adherence rates with prescribed medications is poor [62] and low mood is associated with higher stroke recurrence rates [63] and mortality [1]. Two trials identified in this overview sought to enhance individuals’ beliefs and understanding of the benefits/rationale for preventive actions as their mediator for change [43, 50] but for specific outcomes (e.g. medication adherence), evidence for this TDF domain relies on single study data [43]. However, other research does support the relationship between perceived necessity for medications and adherence rates [56, 57].
In the TDF Emotions domain, three distinct modifiable mediators (coping competence, sense of coherence and trait mindfulness) were identified in the five included trials targeting emotional self-regulation to reduce post-stroke risk [37, 41, 47, 51, 52]. Of these, trait mindfulness, measured using MAAS, demonstrated positive change as the mediator to affect emotional self-regulation behaviour-change in two trials [51, 52]. Mindfulness, a state of awareness and attention to the present, has been shown by systematic review in stroke to derive benefits across psychological, physiological, and psychosocial outcomes including stroke secondary prevention targets of anxiety, depression and blood pressure [64]. Therapeutic benefits of mindfulness for individuals post-stroke, both psychological and physical, are supported by a recent scoping review. However, caveats about knowledge gaps relating to Mindfulness-based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) and the lack of methodological robust studies were highlighted [65].
The Beliefs about Capabilities TDF domain, with self-efficacy as the mediator for behaviour-change, was identified in five trials [38–40, 42, 44] and the Intentions TDF domain, using stages of change as the mediator, was identified in three trials [35, 36, 48]. Whilst self-efficacy was the mediator most frequently employed in primary studies interrogated, no trial employing self-efficacy as the mediator, was associated with health behaviour-changes post-stroke. This finding was unexpected. Self-efficacy is a well-established predictor of behaviour-change relating to physical activity participation for example [10], with a recent prospective cohort study identifying self-efficacy as the strongest contributing factor to stroke/TIA survivors’ intentions to change health behaviours [66]. Perceived self-efficacy is the main causal determinant cited in Social Cognitive Theory [67], is the proposed mediator for change in The Stanford University Chronic Disease Self-Management programme [68] and the Bridges Stroke Self-Management Programme [69]. Whilst individuals’ self-efficacy may have responded positively to the interventions included in the current review, results did not translate to risk-reducing behaviour-change. Likewise, included trials [35, 48] with a mediator anchored in the TDF Intentions domain reported conflicting results between mediators and outcomes. This finding may relate, in part, to poor alignment between theory as a set of concepts and propositions that explain behaviour, and the subsequent use of this theory to then identify and target the determinants of behaviour to affect behaviour-change [27]. This was not clearly articulated in the majority of included trials which also failed to measure all mediators cited in their theory or to link the intervention techniques employed to these constructs, as best practice recommends.
Risk of stroke, stroke recurrence and both short and long-term outcomes disproportionally affect low and middle-income countries [2] where healthcare systems are primarily focused on acute care [70] and inconsistencies in ongoing care and service delivery exist [71]. In addition, deprived or disadvantaged populations within high-income countries where modifiable risk factors are prevalent, experience disparities [72]. Factors such as income, education, social support or isolation, ethnicity, and environmental conditions have been identified as confounding variables that need to be addressed in intervention studies [73, 74]. One study included in the current review showed a positive change in tobacco and alcohol use with African American and Hispanic participants of low socioeconomic status [34]. However, despite the disproportionate burden of stroke on low and middle-income countries, the majority of the papers reviewed originated in high-income countries, limiting the generalisability of the findings. Whilst beyond the scope of this overview, we recognise the need for a better understanding and management of the gap observed between high- and low-income nations when designing and delivering effective stroke secondary prevention interventions.
Overall, results from this overview, which interrogated primary study-level data, need to be interpreted carefully. Despite a rigorous mediator identification process and mapping to an established framework (TDF), an element of subjectivity in this process remains. This is largely dependent on interpreting the definitions and descriptions provided in the primary studies and the mediator measured. Because strict criteria were applied for trial inclusion, evidence identified was often downgraded to low or very low due to the limited number of studies, small sample sizes, and risks of bias. Nevertheless, having GRADE for all included effective interventions is a strength of this overview. The limited number of trials available results, in the main, from unclear reporting of behaviour-change interventions in the RCTs screened, a recurrent issue in the extant literature. The growing number of complex and multicomponent interventions focusing on stroke secondary prevention [75], when recently scrutinised using the updated MRC Framework [20], identified only a small proportion providing a theoretical underpinning, and none reporting their proposed mediators of action or specifying behaviour-change techniques [75]. Studies included in this overview presented wide variations in intervention content and delivery, timing after stroke, duration, location, and outcomes targeted, reflecting many of the challenges inherent in developing and reporting complex interventions in stroke secondary prevention [76]. The small number of trials identified, despite the large volume of SRs, highlights a lack of standardisation in how behaviour-change and self-management interventions are reported, particularly in relation to describing their rationale and theoretical perspectives (points 1 and 2 of the TIDieR checklist [23]).
Future studies should endeavour to describe their behaviour-change interventions in a structured way. This should lay out the use of theory, the theory-related mediator/s targeted to affect behaviour-change, alongside the matched behaviour-change techniques employed. To this end, using TDF domains to select evidence-based behaviour-change techniques (BCT) as the active ingredients of an intervention [77] has evolved with an extensive taxonomy of BCTs available to draw from [78], and recent advances in behaviour-change intervention ontologies to aid the development of effective interventions [79]. Future trials in behaviour-change for stroke secondary prevention, in addition to attending to how the intervention is developed and described, need to include outcomes that measure longer-term cardiovascular outcomes as well as behaviour-change, as these features were markedly absent in the reviewed literature.
Go to:
Conclusion
This overview highlights the current lack of theory-based research and limited use of behaviour-change mediators within stroke secondary prevention trials. The findings identify the theoretical domains of Knowledge, Beliefs about Consequences, and Emotions as having utility in affecting positive changes in risk reducing health behaviours post-stroke to actively increase knowledge and awareness of all aspects of stroke and risk-reducing behaviours; address health beliefs and correct any misperceptions; and support emotional self-regulation. Taken together with the findings from a best-evidence synthesis previously published [16], the active components of successful strategies in theoretically-based behaviour-change and self-management interventions are better explained. Future research should, at a minimum, include these constructs known to be effective, in well conceptualised RCTs, with adequate follow-up time, that include primary outcomes addressing cardiovascular risk in addition to health behaviours.
Go to:
Supporting information
S1 Table
Theoretical Domains Framework.
✔ denotes domains identified in primary studies included in current study.
(DOCX)
Click here to view.(16K, docx)
S2 Table
Preferred reporting guideline for overviews of reviews of healthcare interventions (PRIOR) checklist.
(DOCX)
Click here to view.(23K, docx)
S3 Table
Template for intervention description and replication (TIDieR) checklist.
(DOCX)
Click here to view.(21K, docx)
S4 Table
Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) checklist.
(DOCX)
Click here to view.(33K, docx)
S1 File
Search strategy.
(DOCX)
Click here to view.(26K, docx)
S1 Fig
Model for person-centred stroke secondary prevention behavioural change and self-management.
(TIF)
Click here to view.(1012K, tif)
S2 Fig
Summary of risk of bias.
(TIF)
Click here to view.(863K, tif)
S3 Fig
Risk of bias graph.
Review authors’ judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.
(TIF)
Click here to view.(80K, tif)
S4 Fig
Forest plot: Lifestyle interventions versus usual care for the outcome of mortality.
(TIF)
Click here to view.(166K, tif)
S5 Fig
Forest plot: Lifestyle interventions versus usual care for the outcome of fruit & vegetable consumption.
(TIF)
Click here to view.(87K, tif)
S6 Fig
Forest plot: Lifestyle interventions versus usual care for the outcome of physical activity participation levels.
(TIF)
Click here to view.(78K, tif)
S7 Fig
Forest plot: Lifestyle interventions versus usual care for the outcome of anxiety self-regulation.
(TIF)
Click here to view.(88K, tif)
S8 Fig
Forest plot: Lifestyle interventions versus usual care and/or active control for the outcome of psychological distress.
(TIF)
Click here to view.(104K, tif)
Go to:
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge foundational work by the INSsPiRE network (International Network of Stroke Secondary Prevention Researchers) in identifying and conceptualizing the need for this overview of reviews.
Go to:
Funding Statement
Health Research Board, Ireland, https://www.hrb.ie/ Collaborative Doctoral Award iPASTAR (improving Pathways for Acute Stroke and Rehabilitation) [CDA-2019-004]. The first author (PH) is a PhD scholar funded under this program. Funders did not play any role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Go to:
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files.
Go to:
References
1. Mohan KM, Wolfe CDA, Rudd AG, Heuschmann PU, Kolominsky-Rabas PL, Grieve AP. Risk and Cumulative Risk of Stroke Recurrence. Stroke. 2011;42(5):1489–94. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.602615 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
2. Krishnamurthi RV, Ikeda T, Feigin VL. Global, Regional and Country-Specific Burden of Ischaemic Stroke, Intracerebral Haemorrhage and Subarachnoid Haemorrhage: A Systematic Analysis of the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Neuroepidemiology. 2020;54(2):171–9. doi: 10.1159/000506396 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
3. Talelli P, Greenwood RJ. Review: Recurrent stroke: where do we stand with the secondary prevention of noncardioembolic ischaemic strokes? Therapeutic Advances in Cardiovascular Disease. 2008;2(5):387–405. doi: 10.1177/1753944708093411 . [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
4. O’Donnell MJ, Chin SL, Rangarajan S, Xavier D, Liu L, Zhang H, et al. Global and regional effects of potentially modifiable risk factors associated with acute stroke in 32 countries (INTERSTROKE): a case-control study. The Lancet. 2016;388(10046):761–75. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)30506-2 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
5. Bailey RR, Phad A, McGrath R, Haire-Joshu D. Prevalence of five lifestyle risk factors among US adults with and without stroke. Disability and health journal. 2019;12(2):323–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
6. Hackam DG, Spence JD. Combining multiple approaches for the secondary prevention of vascular events after stroke: a quantitative modeling study. Stroke. 2007;38(6):1881–5. Epub 2007/04/14. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.106.475525 . [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
7. Duncan PW, Bushnell C, Sissine M, Coleman S, Lutz BJ, Johnson AM, et al. Comprehensive stroke care and outcomes: time for a paradigm shift. Stroke. 2021;52(1):385–93. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.120.029678 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
8. Michie S, Abraham C. Identifying techniques that promote health behaviour change: Evidence based or evidence inspired. Psychol Health. 2004;19:29–49. [Google Scholar]
9. Conner M, Norman P. Health behaviour: Current issues and challenges. Psychology & Health. 2017;32(8):895–906. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1336240 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
10. Rhodes RE, Boudreau P, Josefsson KW, Ivarsson A. Mediators of physical activity behaviour change interventions among adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Health psychology review. 2021;15(2):272–86. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2019.1706614 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
11. MacKay-Lyons M, Thornton M, Ruggles T, Che M. Non-pharmacological interventions for preventing secondary vascular events after stroke or transient ischemic attack. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2013;(3). doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008656.pub2 . [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
12. Bridgwood B, Lager KE, Mistri AK, Khunti K, Wilson AD, Modi P. Interventions for improving modifiable risk factor control in the secondary prevention of stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2018;(5). doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009103.pub3 . [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
13. Lawrence M, Pringle J, Kerr S, Booth J, Govan L, Roberts NJ. Multimodal Secondary Prevention Behavioral Interventions for TIA and Stroke: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. PLoS One. 2015;10(3). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120902 . [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
14. Lennon O, Galvin R, Smith K, Doody C, Blake C. Lifestyle interventions for secondary disease prevention in stroke and transient ischaemic attack: a systematic review. European Journal of Preventive Cardiology. 2014;21(8):1026–39. doi: 10.1177/2047487313481756 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
15. Sakakibara BM, Kim AJ, Eng JJ. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis on Self-Management for Improving Risk Factor Control in Stroke Patients. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2017;24(1):42–53. doi: 10.1007/s12529-016-9582-7 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
16. Hall P, Lawrence M, Blake C, Lennon O. Interventions for behaviour change and self-management of risk in stroke secondary prevention: an overview of reviews. Cerebrovascular Diseases. 2023. doi: 10.1159/000531138 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
17. Painter JE, Borba CP, Hynes M, Mays D, Glanz K. The use of theory in health behavior research from 2000 to 2005: a systematic review. Annals of behavioral medicine. 2008;35(3):358–62. doi: 10.1007/s12160-008-9042-y [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
18. Davis R, Campbell R, Hildon Z, Hobbs L, Michie S. Theories of behaviour and behaviour change across the social and behavioural sciences: a scoping review. Health psychology review. 2015;9(3):323–44. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.941722 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
19. Kleindorfer DO, Towfighi A, Chaturvedi S, Cockroft KM, Gutierrez J, Lombardi-Hill D, et al. 2021 Guideline for the prevention of stroke in patients with stroke and transient ischemic attack; A guideline from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke (1970). 2021;52(7):E364–E467. doi: 10.1161/STR.0000000000000375 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
20. Skivington K, Matthews L, Simpson SA, Craig P, Baird J, Blazeby JM, et al. A new framework for developing and evaluating complex interventions: update of Medical Research Council guidance. BMJ. 2021;374:n2061. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n2061 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
21. Francis JJ, O’Connor D, Curran J Theories of behaviour change synthesised into a set of theoretical groupings: introducing a thematic series on the theoretical domains framework. Implementation Science. 2012;7(1):35. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-7-35 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
22. Cane J, O’Connor D, Michie S. Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implementation Science. 2012;7(1):37. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-7-37 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
23. Hoffmann TC, Glasziou PP, Boutron I, Milne R, Perera R, Moher D, et al. Better reporting of interventions: template for intervention description and replication (TIDieR) checklist and guide. BMJ: British Medical Journal. 2014;348(mar07 3):g1687–g. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g1687 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
24. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist GE, Kunz R, Falck-Ytter Y, Alonso-Coello P, et al. GRADE: an emerging consensus on rating quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. Bmj. 2008;336(7650):924–6. Epub 2008/04/26. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39489.470347.AD . [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
25. Gates M, Gates A, Pieper D, Fernandes RM, Tricco AC, Moher D, et al. Reporting guideline for overviews of reviews of healthcare interventions: development of the PRIOR statement. BMJ. 2022;378:e070849. doi: 10.1136/bmj-2022-070849 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
26. Lennon O, Blake C, Booth J, Pollock A, Lawrence M. Interventions for behaviour change and self-management in stroke secondary prevention: protocol for an overview of reviews. Systematic Reviews. 2018;7(1):231. doi: 10.1186/s13643-018-0888-1 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
27. Michie S, Prestwich A. Are Interventions Theory-Based? Development of a Theory Coding Scheme. Health psychology. 2010;29(1):1–8. doi: 10.1037/a0016939 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
28. Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2011;343:d5928. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
29. Review Manager (RevMan) [Computer program]. Version 5.4 ed. The Cochrane Collaboration2020.
30. Deeks JJ, Higgins JP, Altman DG, on behalf of the Cochrane Statistical Methods Group. Analysing data and undertaking meta-analyses. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions 2019. p. 241–84. [Google Scholar]
31. Higgins JP, Eldridge S, Li T. Including variants on randomized trials. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions 2019. p. 569–93. [Google Scholar]
32. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
33. Eames S, Hoffmann T, Worrall L, Read S, Wong A. Randomised controlled trial of an education and support package for stroke patients and their carers. BMJ Open. 2013;3(5):e002538. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-002538 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
34. Evans-Hudnall GL, Stanley MA, Clark AN, Bush AL, Resnicow K, Liu Y, et al. Improving secondary stroke self-care among underserved ethnic minority individuals: a randomized clinical trial of a pilot intervention. J Behav Med. 2014;37(2):196–204. Epub 2012/12/12. doi: 10.1007/s10865-012-9469-2 . [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
35. Gillham S, Endacott R. Impact of enhanced secondary prevention on health behaviour in patients following minor stroke and transient ischaemic attack: a randomized controlled trial. Clinical Rehabilitation. 2010;24(9):822–30. doi: 10.1177/0269215510367970 . [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
36. Green T, Haley E, Eliasziw M, Hoyte K. Education in stroke prevention: efficacy of an educational counselling intervention to increase knowledge in stroke survivors. Can J Neurosci Nurs. 2007;29(2):13–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
37. Hjelle EG, Bragstad LK, Kirkevold M, Zucknick M, Bronken BA, Martinsen R, et al. Effect of a dialogue-based intervention on psychosocial well-being 6 months after stroke in Norway: a randomized controlled trial. J Rehabil Med. 2019;51(8). doi: 10.2340/16501977-2585 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
38. Hoffmann T, Ownsworth T, Eames S, Shum D. Evaluation of brief interventions for enhancing early emotional adjustment following stroke: a pilot randomised controlled trial. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation. 2015;22(2):117–26. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
39. Jones F, Gage H, Drummond A, Bhalla A, Grant R, Lennon S, et al. Feasibility study of an integrated stroke self-management programme: a cluster-randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open. 2016;6(1):e008900. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008900 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
40. Kendall E, Catalano T, Kuipers P, Posner N, Buys N, Charker J. Recovery following stroke: The role of self-management education. Social Science & Medicine. 2007;64(3):735–46. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.09.012 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
41. Kootker JA, Rasquin SM, Lem FC, van Heugten CM, Fasotti L, Geurts AC. Augmented cognitive behavioral therapy for poststroke depressive symptoms: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2017;98(4):687–94. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2016.10.013 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
42. McKenna S, Jones F, Glenfield P, Lennon S. Bridges self-management program for people with stroke in the community: A feasibility randomized controlled trial. Int J Stroke. 2015;10(5):697–704. Epub 2013/11/22. doi: 10.1111/ijs.12195 . [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
43. O’Carroll RE, Chambers JA, Dennis M, Sudlow C, Johnston M. Improving Adherence to Medication in Stroke Survivors: A Pilot Randomised Controlled Trial. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2013;46(3):358–68. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9515-5 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
44. Sit JW, Chair SY, Choi KC, Chan CW, Lee DT, Chan AW, et al. Do empowered stroke patients perform better at self-management and functional recovery after a stroke? A randomized controlled trial. Clin Interv Aging. 2016;11:1441–50. Epub 2016/10/30. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S109560 . [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
45. Sit JW, Yip VY, Ko SK, Gun AP, Lee JS. A quasi-experimental study on a community-based stroke prevention programme for clients with minor stroke. Journal of Clinical Nursing. 2007;16(2):272–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2005.01522.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
46. Slark J, Khan M, Bentley P, Sharma P. Individual risk awareness intervention in stroke (IRAIS): a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Cardiology. 2013;2(1):1017. [Google Scholar]
47. Tielemans NS, Visser-Meily J, Schepers V, van de Passier PE, Port I, Vloothuis J, et al. Effectiveness of the Restore4Stroke self-management intervention “Plan ahead!”: a randomized controlled trial in stroke patients and partners. J Rehabil Med. 2015;47(10):901–9. doi: 10.2340/16501977-2020 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
48. Towfighi A, Cheng EM, Hill VA, Barry F, Lee M, Valle NP, et al. Results of a Pilot Trial of a Lifestyle Intervention for Stroke Survivors: Healthy Eating and Lifestyle after Stroke. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases. 2020;29(12):105323. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2020.105323 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
49. Watkins CL, Auton MF, Deans CF, Dickinson HA, Jack CIA, Lightbody CE, et al. Motivational Interviewing Early After Acute Stroke. Stroke. 2007;38(3):1004–9. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000258114.28006.d7 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
50. Watkins CL, Wathan JV, Leathley MJ, Auton MF, Deans CF, Dickinson HA, et al. The 12-Month Effects of Early Motivational Interviewing After Acute Stroke. Stroke. 2011;42(7):1956–61. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.602227 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
51. Wang X, Li J, Wang C, Lv J. The effects of mindfulness-based intervention on quality of life and poststroke depression in patients with spontaneous intracerebral hemorrhage in China. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2020;35(5):572–80. doi: 10.1002/gps.5273 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
52. Zhang YX, Hao ZW, Guo X. Clinical effect of mindfulness-based behavioral training on post-stroke depression. Chinese Journal of Cardiovascular and Cerebrovascular Diseases of Integrated Traditional and Western Medicine 2015;13(14):1679–81. [Google Scholar]
53. Huang XS, Zou J, Yang MF. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction therapy on anxiety and depression in patients with post-stroke depression. Journal Nursing China. 2017;24:62–4. [Google Scholar]
54. World Bank Country and Lending Groups. The World Bank; 2017 [cited 2014 13/03/2024]. https://datahelpdesk.worldbank.org/knowledgebase/articles/906519.
55. Michie S, Johnston M, Abraham C, Lawton R, Parker D, Walker A. Making psychological theory useful for implementing evidence based practice: a consensus approach. Quality and Safety in Health Care. 2005;14(1):26–33. doi: 10.1136/qshc.2004.011155 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
56. Crayton E, Fahey M, Ashworth M, Besser SJ, Weinman J, Wright AJ. Psychological Determinants of Medication Adherence in Stroke Survivors: a Systematic Review of Observational Studies. Ann Behav Med. 2017;51(6):833–45. Epub 2017/04/20. doi: 10.1007/s12160-017-9906-0 . [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
57. Jamison J, Graffy J, Mullis R, Mant J, Sutton S. Barriers to medication adherence for the secondary prevention of stroke: a qualitative interview study in primary care. British Journal of General Practice. 2016;66(649):e568–e76. doi: 10.3399/bjgp16X685609 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
58. Souter C, Kinnear A, Kinnear M, Mead G. Optimisation of secondary prevention of stroke: a qualitative study of stroke patients’ beliefs, concerns and difficulties with their medicines†. International Journal of Pharmacy Practice. 2014;22(6):424–32. doi: 10.1111/ijpp.12104 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
59. Hall P, von Koch L, Wang X, Lennon O. A Scoping Review of Non-Pharmacological, Non-Surgical Secondary Prevention Strategies in Ischaemic Stroke and TIA in National Stroke Guidelines and Clinical Audit Documents. Healthcare. 2022;10(3):481. doi: 10.3390/healthcare10030481 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
60. Kelly MP, Barker M. Why is changing health-related behaviour so difficult? Public Health. 2016;136:109–16. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2016.03.030 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
61. Crocker TF, Brown L, Lam N, Wray F, Knapp P, Forster A. Information provision for stroke survivors and their carers. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2021;(11). doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001919.pub4 . [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
62. Al AlShaikh S, Quinn T, Dunn W, Walters M, Dawson J. Predictive factors of non-adherence to secondary preventative medication after stroke or transient ischaemic attack: A systematic review and meta-analyses. European stroke journal. 2016;1(2):65–75. doi: 10.1177/2396987316647187 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
63. Wu Q-e, Zhou A-m, Han Y-p, Liu Y-m, Yang Y, Wang X-m, et al. Poststroke depression and risk of recurrent stroke: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. Medicine. 2019;98(42):e17235. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000017235 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
64. Lawrence M, Booth J, Mercer S, Crawford E. A systematic review of the benefits of mindfulness-based interventions following transient ischemic attack and stroke. International Journal of Stroke. 2013;8(6):465–74. doi: 10.1111/ijs.12135 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
65. Mak TC, Wong TW, Ng SS. The use of mindfulness-based interventions in stroke rehabilitation: A scoping review. Rehabilitation Psychology. 2023. doi: 10.1037/rep0000505 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
66. Brouwer-Goossensen D, van Genugten L, Lingsma HF, Dippel DWJ, Koudstaal PJ, den Hertog HM. Self-efficacy for health-related behaviour change in patients with TIA or minor ischemic stroke. Psychology & Health. 2018;33(12):1490–501. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2018.1508686 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
67. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1978;1(4):139–61. doi: 10.1016/0146-6402(78)90002-4 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
68. Lorig KR, Sobel DS, Ritter PL, Laurent D, Hobbs M. Effect of a self-management program on patients with chronic disease. Effective clinical practice: ECP. 2001;4(6):256–62. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
69. Jones F, Mandy A, Partridge C. Changing self-efficacy in individuals following a first time stroke: preliminary study of a novel self-management intervention. Clinical Rehabilitation. 2009;23(6):522–33. doi: 10.1177/0269215508101749 . [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
70. Lindsay P, Furie KL, Davis SM, Donnan GA, Norrving B. World Stroke Organization global stroke services guidelines and action plan. Int J Stroke. 2014;9 Suppl A100:4–13. Epub 2014/09/25. doi: 10.1111/ijs.12371 . [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
71. Chimatiro GL, Rhoda AJ. Scoping review of acute stroke care management and rehabilitation in low and middle-income countries. BMC Health Services Research. 2019;19(1):789. doi: 10.1186/s12913-019-4654-4 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
72. Marshall IJ, Wang Y, Crichton S, McKevitt C, Rudd AG, Wolfe CDA. The effects of socioeconomic status on stroke risk and outcomes. The Lancet Neurology. 2015;14(12):1206–18. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00200-8 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
73. Skolarus LE, Sharrief A, Gardener H, Jenkins C, Boden-Albala B. Considerations in Addressing Social Determinants of Health to Reduce Racial/Ethnic Disparities in Stroke Outcomes in the United States. Stroke. 2020;51(11):3433–9. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.120.030426 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
74. Yadav RS, Chaudhary D, Avula V, Shahjouei S, Azarpazhooh MR, Abedi V, et al. Social Determinants of Stroke Hospitalization and Mortality in United States’ Counties. J Clin Med. 2022;11(14):4101. doi: 10.3390/jcm11144101 . [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
75. Crow J, Savage M, Gardner L, Hughes C, Corbett C, Wells M, et al. What follow-up interventions, programmes and pathways exist for minor stroke survivors after discharge from the acute setting? A scoping review. BMJ Open. 2023;13(6):e070323. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2022-070323 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
76. Redfern J, McKevitt C, Wolfe CDA. Development of Complex Interventions in Stroke Care. Stroke. 2006;37(9):2410–9. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000237097.00342.a9 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
77. Michie S, Johnston M, Francis J, Hardeman W, Eccles M. From Theory to Intervention: Mapping Theoretically Derived Behavioural Determinants to Behaviour Change Techniques. Applied Psychology. 2008;57(4):660–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00341.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
78. Michie S, Richardson M, Johnston M, Abraham C, Francis J, Hardeman W, et al. The Behavior Change Technique Taxonomy (v1) of 93 Hierarchically Clustered Techniques: Building an International Consensus for the Reporting of Behavior Change Interventions. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2013;46(1):81–95. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
79. Michie S, Thomas J, Mac Aonghusa P, West R, Johnston M, Kelly M, et al. The Human Behaviour-Change Project: An artificial intelligence system to answer questions about changing behaviour [version 1; peer review: not peer reviewed]. Wellcome Open Research. 2020;5(122). doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15900.1 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
PLoS One. 2024; 19(4): e0302364. » Decision Letter 0
2024; 19(4): e0302364.
Published online 2024 Apr 26. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0302364.r001
Decision Letter 0
Tinashe Mudzviti, Academic Editor
Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer
23 Feb 2024
PONE-D-23-30783Reducing risk behaviours after stroke: an overview of reviews interrogating primary study data using the Theoretical Domains FrameworkPLOS ONE
Dear Dr. Hall,
Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.
Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 08 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at gro.solp@enosolp. When you’re ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the ‘Submissions Needing Revision’ folder to locate your manuscript file.
Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled ‘Response to Reviewers’.
A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled ‘Revised Manuscript with Track Changes’.
An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled ‘Manuscript’.
If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.
If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.
We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.
Kind regards,
Tinashe Mudzviti, MPhil(MD)
Academic Editor
PLOS ONE
Journal Requirements:
When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.
1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE’s style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at
https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and
https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf
[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]
Reviewers’ comments:
Reviewer’s Responses to Questions
Comments to the Author
1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?
The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.
Reviewer #1: Yes
Reviewer #2: Yes
**********
2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?
Reviewer #1: Yes
Reviewer #2: No
**********
3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?
The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.
Reviewer #1: Yes
Reviewer #2: Yes
**********
4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?
PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.
Reviewer #1: Yes
Reviewer #2: Yes
**********
5. Review Comments to the Author
Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)
Reviewer #1: The manuscript presented for review is articulate and demonstrates adequate methodological rigor.
However, I would like to offer a few constructive comments to enhance its academic merit:
1. SMD definition: You should specify the definition of Standardized Mean Difference (SMD) utilized in your analysis. Given that you have employed RevMan, unless there have been alterations to the default settings, it should be Hedges’s g. Please verify this is the case and appropriately detail it in the methods section.
2. Random Effects Model: In your study, you have implemented a random effects model, employing the Mantel-Haenszel method for dichotomous variables and the Inverse Variance method for continuous variables. This approach is appropriate for the context of your analysis. Nonetheless, it is essential to explicitly state this choice and adequately elucidate its rationale in the methods section.
3. Tabular Representation of Theoretical Framework Domains (TFD):I would suggest to include a table that succinctly encapsulates the TFD. This table should feature, in one column, each domain, and in the adjacent column, a description of these domains as they pertain to your study’s context. Although a similar approach is partially employed in the main text (lines 387-389), it would be beneficial to extend this to encompass domains not found in your study. This will not only provide clarity but also serve as a valuable guide for future research endeavors.
4. Enhanced Detail in Table 1 description of Interventions: The inclusion of an additional column in Table 1, offering a more comprehensive description of each intervention, is advisable for clarity. Presently, the connection between certain components and their corresponding interventions is not immediately clear. Indeed, while certain intervention names are self-explanatory, others are not as immediately apparent in their relationship. For example, the association of the emotional component in Hjelle et al., 2019, remains unclear in the table when only the name of the intervention is provided. To remedy this, I would suggest including a brief description for each intervention in a new column. This addition will clarify the relevance and application of each intervention within the context of your study, ensuring a more comprehensive understanding for the reader and elucidating framework associations.
5. Resolution of Images: The images included in the manuscript in my possess are of low resolution, which detracts from the overall quality of the presentation.
6. High heterogeneity meta-analysis should be reported anyway: Although some meta-analyses may lack validity due to being characterized by high heterogeneity, as reported, it would be interesting and transparent to include them, possibly in the supplementary materials. Moreover, you have indeed reported results with high heterogeneity in lines 312 and 354-359, but these have not been identified as such.
Reviewer #2:
While the manuscript is important to further advance our understanding on stroke, there are several things that the authors should address before it can be published. For my specific comments, see below.
Introduction:
The authors did not explain in detail what is TDF. Why is it important?
Methodology:
The authors did not state what is exclusion criteria for this review. They also did not give justification why they only used three search engines to search the articles. Why did they leave out other search engines like WoS and SCOPUS that might yield more articles? How sure are the authors that comprehensive search has been conducted to make sure no article is missing from this analysis?
Discussion:
Please discuss why most studies included here come from high income nations? Is there any gap observed between high income and low income nations?
Minor correction: Figure presented in manuscript is not clear and hard to read.
**********
6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.
If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.
Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.
Reviewer #1: Yes: Pierfelice Cutrufelli
Reviewer #2: No
**********
[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link “View Attachments”. If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]
While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at gro.solp@serugif. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.
PLoS One. 2024; 19(4): e0302364. » Author response to Decision Letter 0
2024; 19(4): e0302364.
Published online 2024 Apr 26. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0302364.r002
Author response to Decision Letter 0
Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer
16 Mar 2024
Thank you for your positive comments and detailed feedback on our paper. Below is a point-by-point response to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s).
Reviewer #1 Points 1 – 6
Reviewer #1 comments to authors: The manuscript presented for review is articulate and demonstrates adequate methodological rigor. However, I would like to offer a few constructive comments to enhance its academic merit.
Response: We greatly appreciate the positive comments provided in your detailed feedback and thank you for your review.
Point 1
1. SMD definition: You should specify the definition of Standardized Mean Difference (SMD) utilized in your analysis. Given that you have employed RevMan, unless there have been alterations to the default settings, it should be Hedges’s g. Please verify this is the case and appropriately detail it in the methods section.
Response: Thank you, the SMD used in the analysis is Hedges’ g and we have detailed this in the data synthesis of the methods section which now reads:
Meta-analyses, conducted using Review Manager 5 (RevMan5)[29], grouped data by TDF domains and secondary prevention outcomes, where data presented permitted. For continuous data, where different scales assessed the same outcome, standardized mean differences (SMD) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated. SMD is used as a summary statistic to measure effect size that quantifies differences in standard deviations between two groups. The Hedges’ g version of SMD conducted here in RevMan5 is the preferred statistic when sample sizes are unequal and/or are small (< 20), as the case in the current study, as it takes each sample size into consideration when calculating the overall effect size.
Point 2
2. Random Effects Model: In your study, you have implemented a random effects model, employing the Mantel-Haenszel method for dichotomous variables and the Inverse Variance method for continuous variables. This approach is appropriate for the context of your analysis. Nonetheless, it is essential to explicitly state this choice and adequately elucidate its rationale in the methods section.
Response: Thank you we have amended the methods section to provide the necessary clarity. This section now reads in the manuscript:
For continuous data where different scales assessed the same outcome, standardized mean differences (SMD) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated. SMD is used as a summary statistic to measure effect size that quantifies differences in standard deviations between two groups. The Hedges’ g version of SMD conducted here in RevMan5 is the preferred statistic when sample sizes are unequal and/or are small (< 20), as the case in the current study, as it takes each sample size into consideration when calculating the overall effect size. The inverse variance method was used as it is especially suitable when using SMD to minimise uncertainty of the overall effect size[30]. For dichotomous variables, odds ratios (OR) with 95% CIs were employed using the Mantel-Haenszel method. Random effects models were applied to provide a more conservative estimate of overall effect size as statistical heterogeneity was assumed[30]. The I2 statistic measured heterogeneity; >50% was considered substantial.
Point 3
3. Tabular Representation of Theoretical Framework Domains (TFD): I would suggest to include a table that succinctly encapsulates the TFD. This table should feature, in one column, each domain, and in the adjacent column, a description of these domains as they pertain to your study’s context. Although a similar approach is partially employed in the main text (lines 387-389), it would be beneficial to extend this to encompass domains not found in your study. This will not only provide clarity but also serve as a valuable guide for future research endeavors.
Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We have now included a TDF table in the supplemental file and referenced this in the introduction which now reads:
Comprising 87 component parts across fourteen overarching domains of Knowledge, Skills, Social/Professional Role and Identity, Beliefs about Capabilities, Optimism, Beliefs about Consequences, Reinforcement, Intentions, Goals, Memory, Attention and Decision Processes, Environmental Context and Resources, Social Influences, Emotions, and Behavioural Regulation[22], the TDF provides comprehensive coverage of the possible mediators influencing behaviour-change. Tabular representation of the TDF describes these domains as they pertain to stroke secondary prevention in the current study (supplemental file, S1 Table).
Point 4
4. Enhanced Detail in Table 1 description of Interventions: The inclusion of an additional column in Table 1, offering a more comprehensive description of each intervention, is advisable for clarity. Presently, the connection between certain components and their corresponding interventions is not immediately clear. Indeed, while certain intervention names are self-explanatory, others are not as immediately apparent in their relationship. For example, the association of the emotional component in Hjelle et al., 2019, remains unclear in the table when only the name of the intervention is provided. To remedy this, I would suggest including a brief description for each intervention in a new column. This addition will clarify the relevance and application of each intervention within the context of your study, ensuring a more comprehensive understanding for the reader and elucidating framework associations.
Response: Thank you for this suggestion. An additional column has been inserted in the characteristics table to provide a brief description of each intervention to aid clarity. The characteristics table headings now read as below with additional column (full table not included here but title row copied below for your convenience).
Study Participants Intervention theoretical perspectives Brief description Mediator TDF Domain Intervention Time to follow-up Outcomes measured Key findings
Point 5
5. Resolution of Images: The images included in the manuscript in my possess are of low resolution, which detracts from the overall quality of the presentation.
Response: Thank you. All images will now be uploaded to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) as now advised to ensure quality of figures.
Point 6
6. High heterogeneity meta-analysis should be reported anyway: Although some meta-analyses may lack validity due to being characterized by high heterogeneity, as reported, it would be interesting and transparent to include them, possibly in the supplementary materials. Moreover, you have indeed reported results with high heterogeneity in lines 312 and 354-359, but these have not been identified as such.
Response: Thank you. We have included all meta-analyses conducted either directly in the manuscript or in supplemental material irrespective of heterogeneity identified. Where some confusion may have arisen is where studies employed different outcome measures that did not allow meta-analyses to be conducted for example – salt and salty food consumption; alcohol abstinence, units consumed per week, percentage of self-reported alcohol reduction. We previously reported this as “heterogeneity in reported outcomes prohibited meta-analysis”.
We have reworded this in the manuscript in the Diet and the Alcohol consumption section of the Results which now reads: The units or mode of measurement employed did not allow data to be pooled.
We have clarified in the methods section the level of heterogeneity considered which now reads:
The I2 statistic measured heterogeneity; >50% was considered substantial[30].
In addition, in the results section where we have reported results with high heterogeneity, we have now identified these as so. The manuscript now reads:
Physical activity participation
Results demonstrated no significant effect in favour of the intervention group (SMD -0.07 [-0.46, 0.32], p = 0.72, I2 = 67%), however heterogeneity was substantial (S6 Fig).
Depression
Data permitted meta-analysis from four trials using behaviour-change mediators mapped to TDF domains of Beliefs about Capabilities [37, 38] and Emotions[50, 51], demonstrating evidence of effect in favour of the intervention (SMD -0.70 [-1.28, -0.12], p = 0.02, I2 = 81%), with high heterogeneity (Fig 5e). Evidence of effect in the TDF Emotions domain, drawn from 2 trials (SMD -0.99 [-1.97, -0.00], p=0.05, I2 = 91%) is also evident, however heterogeneity was considerable.
Reviewer #2 Comments 1 – 4
Reviewer #2 comments to authors: While the manuscript is important to further advance our understanding on stroke, there are several things that the authors should address before it can be published. For my specific comments, see below.
Response: We very much appreciate your positive comments and thank you for your review. Outlined below are point-to-point responses to each specific comment.
Comment 1 Introduction:
The authors did not explain in detail what is TDF. Why is it important?
Response: Thank you. We have amended the introduction to include further details to explain the TDF and it’s importance, and have included a TDF table in a supplemental file to provide greater clarity, referenced in the introduction which now reads:
Explicit use of theory in the design and evaluation of interventions in stroke secondary prevention presents this opportunity to understand why interventions work, for whom, and in what context[18]. Selecting one or more theories as the basis for intervention development can prove challenging, partly due to often overlapping theoretical constructs[21]. The Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) is a comprehensive theory-informed approach to understanding the determinants of behaviour change and the factors influencing intervention development. The TDF was developed to allow theories and their constructs to be synthesised into groupings to make behaviour-change theories more accessible in intervention design and analysis[21, 22]. This is important as it provides a systematic and rigorous framework for understanding behaviour change in multiple populations and settings. Comprising 87 component parts across fourteen overarching domains of Knowledge, Skills, Social/Professional Role and Identity, Beliefs about Capabilities, Optimism, Beliefs about Consequences, Reinforcement, Intentions, Goals, Memory, Attention and Decision Processes, Environmental Context and Resources, Social Influences, Emotions, and Behavioural Regulation[22], the TDF provides comprehensive coverage of the possible mediators influencing behaviour-change. Tabular representation of the TDF describes these domains as they pertain to stroke secondary prevention in the current study (supplemental file, S1 Table).
Comment 2 Methodology:
The authors did not state what is exclusion criteria for this review.
Response: Thank you for your feedback. Apologies, we stated the exclusion criteria in the screening and selection section and have amended the manuscript by moving this to explicitly state it following the inclusion criteria. The manuscript now reads:
Inclusion/Exclusion criteria
SRs of randomized control trials (RCTs) or cluster RCTs (CRCT) testing interventions for behaviour-change and/or self-management of risk in stroke secondary prevention were first identified. Primary studies included in these reviews were then considered where the following were detailed:
• Adult population comprising stroke/TIA
• Intervention/s targeting stroke risk reduction at an individual or population level
• Intervention/s identifying a theoretical perspective and measuring a stated mediator for behaviour-change that mapped to the TDF
• Comparators of usual care, placebo, sham, or other intervention
• Outcomes recorded that addressed mortality, recurrent stroke or other cardiovascular events, or secondary outcomes addressing any one or combination of the following health behaviours – secondary prevention medication adherence, healthy diet, physical activity participation, smoking cessation, safe alcohol consumption and emotional self-regulation.
Exclusion criteria applied:
• Interventions designed to alter care process or health professionals’ education/practice.
• Interventions not targeting behaviour-change in stroke secondary prevention.
• Telehealth interventions
• Interventions targeting family/partner dyads, unless behaviour-change in the person with stroke was specifically targeted and extractable.
They also did not give justification why they only used three search engines to search the articles. Why did they leave out other search engines like WoS and SCOPUS that might yield more articles? How sure are the authors that comprehensive search has been conducted to make sure no article is missing from this analysis?
Response: Thank you. We worked closely with the information scientist (librarian) on the search strings using controlled vocabulary and free text terms relating to stroke and secondary prevention/lifestyle risk behaviour which we developed and adapted for the included databases – Epistemonikos, Cochrane Library of Systematic Reviews, Medline, Embase. These libraries were chosen as they represent two systematic review databases and two of the largest health focused databases. For the databases not specific to systematic reviews (Medline, Embase), a third methodological search string for systematic reviews was included. Based on our experience and previous searches we were confident these databases would contain the reviews we were looking for. As an overview of systematic reviews only primary studies included in these reviews were included therefore it is possible that more recent RCTs are not included.
To clarify this in the Search strategy section the manuscript now reads:
Using a comprehensive search strategy compiled in conjunction with a liaison librarian, electronic databases MEDLINE, Embase, Epistemonikos and Cochrane Library of Systematic Reviews were systematically searched from inception to March 2023 with no limitations applied. For databases not specific to systematic reviews (Medline, Embase), a third methodological search string for systematic reviews was included. These two databases were chosen as they are two of the largest health focussed databases and we were confident, based on our experience and previous searches that they would contain the reviews we were looking for. In addition, reference lists of included SRs were checked. It is possible that more recent RCTs, not yet reviewed in SRs are not included. The full search strategy which targeted published systematic reviews is provided (S1 File).
Comment 3 Discussion:
Please discuss why most studies included here come from high income nations? Is there any gap observed between high income and low income nations?
Response: Thank you for your comments and support for this paper. We welcome your observations related to the significant gap in the global burden of stroke between high-income and low-income nations. Our review purposely focused on the available literature from identified systematic reviews, of theoretically-grounded interventions that applied mediators for behaviour-change. Despite the disproportionate burden of stroke on low and middle income countries, the majority of the papers originated in high-income countries.
We have clarified this in the description of the included RCTs in the results section which now reads:
Table 1 summarises the characteristics of all nineteen included RCTs. The trials were conducted across four geographical locations – Australia[32, 37, 39], North America[33, 35, 47], Asia[43, 44, 50, 51] and Europe[34, 36, 38, 40-42, 45, 46, 49]. When broken down by country and world bank classification all but two studies from an upper-middle-income economy (China)[51, 52] originated in high-income nations[54].
Response: thank you for your feedback. We now address this observed gap in the discussion section which reads in the manuscript:
Risk of stroke, stroke recurrence and both short and long-term outcomes disproportionally affect low and middle-income countries[2] where healthcare systems are primarily focused on acute
Attachment
Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx
Click here to view.(59K, docx)
PLoS One. 2024; 19(4): e0302364. » Decision Letter 1
2024; 19(4): e0302364.
Published online 2024 Apr 26. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0302364.r003
Decision Letter 1
Tinashe Mudzviti, Academic Editor
Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer
3 Apr 2024
Reducing risk behaviours after stroke: an overview of reviews interrogating primary study data using the Theoretical Domains Framework
PONE-D-23-30783R1
Dear Dr. Hall,
We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.
Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.
An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information’ link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at gro.solp@gnillibrohtua.
If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible — no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact gro.solp@sserpeno.
Kind regards,
Tinashe Mudzviti, MPhil(MD)
Academic Editor
PLOS ONE
Additional Editor Comments (optional):
Reviewers’ comments:
Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS
OTHER FORMATS
PDF (2.1M)
ACTIONS
Cite
Collections
SHARE
RESOURCES
Similar articles
Cited by other articles
Links to NCBI Databases
FOLLOW NCBI
Connect with NLM
National Library of Medicine
8600 Rockville Pike
Bethesda, MD 20894
Web Policies
FOIA
HHS Vulnerability Disclosure
Help
Accessibility
Careers
NLM
NIH
HHS
USA.gov
Recent News posts
Exiting news- The iPASTAR International Conference Programme is now available!
PROGRAMME Stroke for the future: The Vision for Comprehensive Stroke Care Pathways November 6th- RCSI University of Medicine…
iPASTAR International Conference- Stroke For The Future 2024
The iPASTAR International Conference and Symposium, which will take place at RCSI Dublin on Wednesday, November 6, 2024,…
Poster Presentations at the 10th European Stroke Organisation Conference (ESOC) May 2024
Geraldine O Callaghan and Patricia Hall presented their posters at the European Life after Stroke Conference in Basel,…
iPASTAR International Conference and Symposium November 6, 2024.
Exciting news! Stroke for the Future- The vision for Comprehensive Stroke Care Pathways Conference, will take place on…